Neuroscience

Articles and news from the latest research reports.

277 notes

Babies develop conscious perception from five months of age
Infants develop the ability to consciously process their environment as early as five months of age, according to a study published in the journal Science.
The team of French and Danish researchers, led by neuroscientist Sid Kouider, discovered a signal in the nervous system of infants that reliably identifies the beginning of visual consciousness, or the ability to see something and recall that you have seen it.
The team set out believing infants had the capacity for conscious reflection, but they had to overcome the barrier that babies could not report their thoughts.
They used electroencephalography (EEG) to record electrical activity in the brains of 80 infants aged five, 12 and 15 months while they were shown pictures of faces and random patterns for a fraction of a second.
When adults are aware of a stimulus, their brains show a two-stage pattern of activity. When they see a moving object, the sensors in the vision centre of the brain activate with a spike of activity.
The signal then moves from the back of the brain to the prefrontal cortex, which deals with higher-level cognition. This is known as the late slow wave.
Conscious awareness begins after the second stage of neural activity reaches a specific threshold.
The new study found this two-stage pattern of brain activity was present in the three groups of infants, though it was weaker and more drawn out in the five-month-olds.
The researchers say neurological markers of visual consciousness may help paediatricians better manage infant pain and anaesthesia.
But they note the research does not provide direct proof of consciousness. “Indeed, it is a genuine philosophical problem whether such a proof can ever be obtained from purely neurophsysiological data,” the paper said.
Professor Louise Newman, Director of the Centre for Developmental Psychiatry & Psychology at Monash University, said the study was novel in its ability to measure the way very young brains register stimuli.
But five months should not be seen as a fixed point at which infants start to process information, she said.
“Although this group has studied five months and up, my suspicion would be that if we had different techniques, young infants – from birth on – would show the capacity of registering these sorts of stimuli.
“Infants are born with quite sophisticated capacities to observe, respond to and interact with the environment, particularly the social environment,” she said.
“Very soon after birth, infants will maintain gaze with their parents or parent: they’ve got quite sophisticated visual tracking capacity from an early age.”
Professor Newman, who has undertaken behavioural studies in two- to four-month olds, said young infant brains were extremely sensitive to their mother’s emotional reaction.
“They learn that ‘if I do this, or if I smile or signal in this way, this is what usually happens’. If you manipulate that so they don’t get that response, they’re very sensitive to that and they show signs that it’s very aversive to them.”

Babies develop conscious perception from five months of age

Infants develop the ability to consciously process their environment as early as five months of age, according to a study published in the journal Science.

The team of French and Danish researchers, led by neuroscientist Sid Kouider, discovered a signal in the nervous system of infants that reliably identifies the beginning of visual consciousness, or the ability to see something and recall that you have seen it.

The team set out believing infants had the capacity for conscious reflection, but they had to overcome the barrier that babies could not report their thoughts.

They used electroencephalography (EEG) to record electrical activity in the brains of 80 infants aged five, 12 and 15 months while they were shown pictures of faces and random patterns for a fraction of a second.

When adults are aware of a stimulus, their brains show a two-stage pattern of activity. When they see a moving object, the sensors in the vision centre of the brain activate with a spike of activity.

The signal then moves from the back of the brain to the prefrontal cortex, which deals with higher-level cognition. This is known as the late slow wave.

Conscious awareness begins after the second stage of neural activity reaches a specific threshold.

The new study found this two-stage pattern of brain activity was present in the three groups of infants, though it was weaker and more drawn out in the five-month-olds.

The researchers say neurological markers of visual consciousness may help paediatricians better manage infant pain and anaesthesia.

But they note the research does not provide direct proof of consciousness. “Indeed, it is a genuine philosophical problem whether such a proof can ever be obtained from purely neurophsysiological data,” the paper said.

Professor Louise Newman, Director of the Centre for Developmental Psychiatry & Psychology at Monash University, said the study was novel in its ability to measure the way very young brains register stimuli.

But five months should not be seen as a fixed point at which infants start to process information, she said.

“Although this group has studied five months and up, my suspicion would be that if we had different techniques, young infants – from birth on – would show the capacity of registering these sorts of stimuli.

“Infants are born with quite sophisticated capacities to observe, respond to and interact with the environment, particularly the social environment,” she said.

“Very soon after birth, infants will maintain gaze with their parents or parent: they’ve got quite sophisticated visual tracking capacity from an early age.”

Professor Newman, who has undertaken behavioural studies in two- to four-month olds, said young infant brains were extremely sensitive to their mother’s emotional reaction.

“They learn that ‘if I do this, or if I smile or signal in this way, this is what usually happens’. If you manipulate that so they don’t get that response, they’re very sensitive to that and they show signs that it’s very aversive to them.”

Filed under infants visual consciousness EEG brain activity perception consciousness neuroscience science

  1. druudawn reblogged this from neurosciencestuff
  2. brainangle reblogged this from neurosciencestuff
  3. pappa-seahorse reblogged this from neurosciencestuff
  4. xinf1nity reblogged this from sagansense
  5. katwitha-k reblogged this from neurosciencestuff
  6. pinkiepieaddict reblogged this from starsaremymuse
  7. ahau-olga reblogged this from neurosciencestuff
  8. geeky-book-nerd reblogged this from mypocketshurt90
  9. fourfourteen reblogged this from astro-stoner
  10. sargazms reblogged this from astro-stoner
  11. newfriendly reblogged this from somuchscience
  12. astro-stoner reblogged this from somuchscience
  13. cmartemis14 reblogged this from somuchscience
  14. neepsterdoofus reblogged this from somuchscience and added:
    This is so important.
  15. zeldainable reblogged this from somuchscience
  16. detailsparklehut reblogged this from somuchscience
  17. somuchscience reblogged this from sagansense
  18. clockworkpancake reblogged this from mypocketshurt90
  19. mrmock reblogged this from mypocketshurt90
  20. laprofesorarevolucionaria reblogged this from radiantvoice
  21. thesethgroup reblogged this from neurosciencestuff
  22. valswei reblogged this from mypocketshurt90
  23. kommgehwech reblogged this from mypocketshurt90
  24. turquoiseheartsofconstellations reblogged this from mypocketshurt90
  25. k-tony reblogged this from mypocketshurt90
  26. mypocketshurt90 reblogged this from neurosciencestuff
  27. heavenbegatshell reblogged this from sagansense
  28. marcusaquarius reblogged this from anthrocentric
  29. uma-carta-para-deus reblogged this from anthrocentric and added:
    mi empresta esse olhos!!
free counters